Table of Contents
Send a Letter
Back Issues
Magazine Staff
 
Departments
Editors's Notes
Letters
Investigations
Chicago Journal
Class News
Books by Alumni
Deaths
Sketchbook
 
Citations
For the Record
Center Stage
Ad Infinitum
Voices
 
Alumni Gateway
UofC Homepage
 

Innate Gestures

As an amateur potter shaping clay or as a professional linguist studying gestures, U of C professor Susan Goldin-Meadow is fascinated by what the hands can create.

Goldin-Meadow and project researcher Carolyn Mylander reported in the January 15 issue of Nature that the human brain has an inherent ability to organize symbols of language and that structured human communication is inevitable, re-gardless of cultural differences.

They based their conclusions on a study of deaf children in the United States and Taiwan who had independently developed similar gesture systems—systems that reflect the language structures of neither English nor Mandarin.

“I was interested in the question of where language comes from and what role a language model might play in a child’s acquisition of language,” says Goldin-Meadow, a 48-year-old professor in psychology, education, the Center for East Asian Studies, and the College. “While a child in normal circumstances gets lots of input, I wanted to see what a child could do without so much input—if the child would, in a sense, reinvent the wheel.”

Goldin-Meadow, who joined the Chicago faculty in 1976 after receiving a Ph.D. in psychology from the University of Pennsylvania, knew from earlier research that American deaf children who had had no contact with each other developed similar signals. But she wanted to study a cross-cultural example and to determine whether the children’s gestures resembled natural language.

“You can get your points across by drawing pictures or taking your mom’s hand and dragging her to the refrigerator and whining,” she explains. “But I was interested in something a bit more symbolic.”

First, the researchers had to find subjects with little exposure to conventional ways of communicating. They turned to deaf children whose hearing losses were so severe that they could not acquire the spoken language around them, and whose hearing parents had not yet taught them sign language.

They then analyzed some 80 hours of videotape that captured more than 10,000 individual gestures between eight selected children and their mothers. Three children lived in Chicago, one in Philadelphia, and four in Taipei, chosen because mothers there interact much differently with their children than do those in America.

All of the children were enrolled in schools that advocated training in sound sensitivity, lip reading, and speech production, and they were observed twice between the ages of 3 years, 8 months and 4 years, 11 months.

The researchers found that the pre-schoolers in both countries developed complex gesture sentences on their own. While two-gesture sentences were the most common in both cultures, all of the children produced sentences up to 13 gestures long, says Goldin-Meadow.

They typically put the object of the verb before the verb—a grammar different from that of both English and Mandarin, she says. For example, she explains, if a deaf child gestured “boy hit,” the child likely meant that the boy was hit rather than that he was the hitter. Educators can use this knowledge, she says, as a base for further instruction.

Observes Goldin-Meadow, “Given the salient differences between Chinese and American cultures, the structural similarities in the children’s gesture systems are striking.”

—Betsy Rossen Elliot