Investigations
Pop starlings and their
melodies
Chicago researchers are peering inside the minds
of European starlings to learn how they recognize songs—and
in the process are providing insights into how the brain learns,
recognizes, and remembers complex sounds at the cellular level.
In the August 7 Nature Timothy Gentner, organismal biology
& anatomy research associate, and Daniel Margoliash, professor
of organismal biology & anatomy and psychology, show how songs
that birds have learned to recognize trigger responses both in individual
neurons and in groups of neurons in the birds’ brains.
Photo by Daniel Baleckaitis |
Songbirds
recall motifs in each others’ tunes. Chicago biologists
study the recollection mechanism in birds’ brains, hoping
to shed light on human memory. |
“We found that cells in a part of the brain
are altered dramatically by the learning process,” Margoliash
says. “As birds learn to recognize certain songs, the cells
in this area”—the cmHV, analogous to the higher-order,
secondary auditory cortex in humans—“become sensitive
to particular sound patterns or auditory objects that occur in the
learned songs, while cells never show such sensitivity to patterns
in unfamiliar songs. Specific cells in the brain become ‘tuned’
to what the bird is learning.”
How the brain perceives and interprets stimuli
from the external world are fundamental neuroscience questions.
Memories of words, sounds of voices, or patterns of music are important
components of human daily experience and are essential for much
communication, yet, Margoliash says, “we know little about
how such memories are formed in the brain and how they are retrieved.”
Bird songs have captured human interest for ages.
As Margoliash notes, “Birders can often recognize many species
of birds by only their songs.” For birds, however, the ability
to match a singer to a song—often down to the level of an
individual bird—can mean the difference between “a day
spent wrestling through the thicket and one spent enjoying a sun-soaked
perch,” Margoliash says, “or the missed chance at mating
with the healthiest partner around.”
Gentner, the study’s lead investigator,
has tapped into starlings’ recognition abilities by training
about 30 birds to identify songs. The birds, brought from the wild
to the lab as adults, were taught to peck different buttons on a
small metal panel to indicate specific starling tunes from computer
recordings. The researchers rewarded correct responses with food
and conveyed incorrect responses by turning off the lights.
Gentner's earlier research has shown that European
starlings learn to recognize different songs by the individual pieces
that comprise each tune. “If you listen closely to a singing
starling,” Gentner says, “you'll hear that the song
is really composed of much shorter sounds. We call these sounds
‘motifs,’ and to produce a song the bird will sing the
same motif a few times, then switch to a new repeated motif, and
then another, as long as he can keep it going. When male starlings
sing, they might use only half of the motifs they know and then
mix up the motifs when they sing another song.”
Given this highly variable structure, when other
starlings learn which songs belong to which individuals, they do
so by concentrating on the motifs. “Even one or two familiar
motifs in an otherwise unfamiliar song,” Gentner says, “is
enough to trigger recognition.”
To examine the neural mechanisms associated with auditory memory,
he and Margoliash measured cmHV-region nerve cells’ electrical
impulses in starlings trained to recognize several songs. The researchers
recorded each neuron’s electrical response to songs the birds
had learned to recognize, to new songs, and to synthetic sounds
such as white noise.
As a group the cells responded much more strongly
to the songs the birds had learned to recognize than to the other
sounds. Individually most cells responded to only one song, and
almost all (93 percent) of these cells responded to a song the bird
had learned to recognize. After examining the data even closer,
the researchers found that many of these cells responded only to
specific motifs within the song.
“The song motifs that drive these cells
so strongly are the same components of sounds that control recognition
behavior in the birds,” Gentner explains. “It appears
that we are seeing the memory traces for recognition of these complex
acoustic patterns. Rather than representing all motifs equally well
at any time, we find that experience modifies the brain to highlight
those motifs that are the most important to the bird at that time.”
Why specific motifs are critical at a certain time is still unknown,
and a question Gentner plans to study.
He and Margoliash also plan to look deeper
into starlings’ capacity for memory. “Memories are not
permanent," Margoliash notes. “Do we lose memories because
of disuse or because they are crowded out by other memories? Our
research shows that the context in which you learn a sound affects
how it is memorized. What are the brain mechanisms that control
this process of how a memory is laid down?"—Catherine
Gianaro
|